The ability to determine rank amongst monk parakeets appears to be an act of cognitive reasoning (photo by Greg Matthews)

A new paper in PLoS Computational Biology by SFI External Professor Simon DeDeo and Elizabeth Hobson of the National Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis sheds light on how socially complex animal societies evolve, and how dominance hierarchies are established. 

Their study of aggression in monk parakeets suggests that where the birds stand in the pecking order is a function of their carefully calibrated perceptions of the ranks of their fellow-feathered friends. 

“Rank and dominance matter — not just for humans, but for the animals as well," says DeDeo. "Rank is not just about whether, say, people think you are high status; it is also whether or not the people who others think are high status think you are high status. That kind of thinking is a sign of a complex social system.”

Newly formed groups of monk parakeets do not show evidence that they perceive rank, yet an awareness of it emerges quickly, after about a week of interactions, which is when individuals direct aggression more frequently against those nearby in rank rather than with lower-ranked birds.

But how do the birds infer rank – their own and the ranks of those in the rest of the flock – and then act upon it?

“Parakeets appear to be able to connect the dots in their groups, remembering chains of aggression, so if A fights B, then watches how B fights C and how C fights D and how D fights E, then A will use this knowledge to adjust how it interacts with E based on all of the fights in between,” says the study’s lead author Elizabeth Hobson, a postdoctoral fellow at the National Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis.

A socially and cognitively complex species, monk parakeets inhabit a social structure organized by dominance hierarchies, such that each animal is ranked as dominant over animals below it and submissive to those above it in the hierarchy. Aggressive encounters usually drive rankings, and a higher rank, of course, often means better foraging and greater chances of reproductive success.

Unlike other animals that might use visual cues (such as size) or perceptional clues (such as spatial location) to determine rank, the monk parakeet appears to rely on other clues – ones that are based on newly acquired social knowledge, the study found.

DeDeo and Hobson analyzed detailed observations of aggression in two independent groups of captive monk parakeets. Each group was observed over the course of 24 days.

“We looked for cases where we could clearly determine a winner when aggression took place – often these were cases where a bird approached another and knocked it off its perch. It’s pretty easy to determine the winner in these kinds of events,” Hobson said.

A total of 1013 wins in one group and 1360 wins in the second group were analyzed. Mathematically sophisticated computation with this rich dataset then revealed that as individuals begin to interact and watch the fights of others, they accumulate knowledge of who wins in fights against whom. Once this knowledge is present, the birds use it refine their own behavior, for example, by avoiding fights with birds higher-ranked than themselves, but also birds that were much lower-ranked. Instead, they focused their aggression by choosing individuals with whom they might be closely matched. In effect, the aggression becomes more strategically directed.

Thus, the ability to determine rank amongst these socially precocious birds appears to be an act of cognitive complexity, learned through the bird’s careful observation of how the other birds interact.

“Our approach provides insight not only into how these parakeets are behaving in their groups, but also into the cognitive skills they would need to exhibit these kinds of strategic behaviors," Hobson adds. "This allows us to start to understand the interaction between social and cognitive complexity and to begin to compare what we see in the parakeet groups to other socially complex species like primates."

“What we’re able to trace is the emergence not just of a class system, if you will, but of the awareness of class by the animals themselves," adds DeDeo. "Given this awareness, they can act in ways that can both re-enforce, and challenge, the hierarchies they find themselves in. While most individuals allow themselves to be guided by the rank system, we also see 'entrepreneurs'—individuals breaking the rules to challenge those higher up."

Text by Catherine Crawley of the National Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis 

Read the paper in PLoS Computational Biology (September 10, 2015)

Read the article in Scientific Computing (September 11, 2015)

Read the article on Audubon.org (September 10, 2015)

Listen to Simon DeDeo's interview podcast on People Behind the Science (September 7, 2015)